Language as a Social Phenomenon (12-Jan-2004)

A conversation between individuals contains information not only in the basic meaning of the words spoken, but also can signal other information about the speakers, situation, and the relationship between the participants:

Certain types of speech and accent have associations with social status, and individuals may vary (consciously or not) the way that they speak. Labov was one of the first researchers to demonstrate that this occurs: he showed that New Yorkers regarded the articulation of the non-prevocalic "r" sound (i.e. an "r" that doesn't come before a vowel, e.g. "car", "park", "guard") as a "prestige feature" (Trudgill p.11), but also that a given individual would use or omit the sound, depending on the situation.

Studies looking at the way that the "h" is dropped at the start of words (e.g. "'ouse" vs. "house") have found similar results: the lower social class the speaker, the more likely he is to drop "h". But regardless of social class, the "h" is less likely to be dropped the more formal the context - i.e. the same person will not always either drop or not drop the "h" sound. This suggests that this variation in pronunciation may be a way to signal solidarity with other people of the same class.

In RP English, the words "could" and "cud" are pronounced as could and cud, but in parts of Northern England the vowel used is the same (both are pronounced cud). This usage seems to be declining though, while the distinction between the "a" vowel sound e.g. in the word "last" is regarded as a source of pride: RP uses lahst, while Northern England has last.

Key Points (from handout)

Book readings:


Language in the Individual and in Society notes index