Language and Gender (23-Feb-2004)
Absolute Differences between Men and Women
For the purposes of linguistics, sex and gender are separate, albeit
overlapping, ideas: sex refers to biological, or physiological
differences between men and women, and can have have an effect on linguistic
processes - e.g. men tend to have voices of a lower pitch than women. In some
cases people may raise or deepen their voices to sound more feminie or
masculine. Gender refers to the stereotypes that go along with social
perceptions of the roles for men and women. Gender also has a
grammatical connotation, with certain languages having "masculine" and
"feminine" forms for certain words.
Some languages have different words for the same thing depending on whether
the speaker is male or female (see handout for examples). There is evidence
that the female forms are "older" forms, which suggests that women are more
conservative then men in adopting new word forms.
In languages such as French, a speaker will say different things depending
on the sex of the speaker or the referent:
- "je suis heureux" (male speaker) vs. "je suis heureuse" (female
speaker)
- "tu es beau" vs. "tu es belle"
- "il s'est assis" vs. "elle s'est assise"
The only remnant of this in English is the third pronoun "he/she", and this is
only used in reference to the sex of the person being referred to - other
languages, e.g. French, German, use these forms for objects which to an
English person have no gender (tables, chairs).
There do seem to be gender assumptions in our perception of
prototypes. Asked to imagine "a student", more people picture a male
student than is justified by the real ratio of male/female students. The kind
of words we use affects this perception, which is one reason why words such as
"fireman" are replaced, e.g. with "firefighter": the original word leads you
to expect that the prototypical firefighter is a man, and that women are
inferior or invalid subtypes. There also seems to be a general perception of
the prototypical human as being a man, with women being classified as "an
extra".
Sex-preferential use
Research in different societies consistently shows that "prestige" features of
language (e.g. postvocalic /r/ in Detroit) are not only more frequent in
higher social classes across genders, but that in all classes women seem to
use the feature more than the men in any given class. In other words, women
seem to speak like the men in the next social class up. This seems to imply
some kind of aspirational aspect to their speech.
Trudgill suggests that these findings may be explicable in terms of
overt and covert prestige (see Trudgill p74-77)
- overt prestige refers to norms of speech which people are consciously
aware of, e.g. the fact that "ain't" is not standard English; when people
consciously avoid "ain't" because they believe it is "incorrect", this is an
example of overt prestige
- covert prestige refers to the use of "non-standard" forms in order to
raise prestige with their peers, or to indicate rebellion against authority
etc..
When asked to rate their own performance, Trudgill found that women are more
likely to over-report their use of non-standard features, and men more likely
to under-report. Additionally, men use non-standard features less than
women. Trudgill cites this as evidence that men feel covert prestige
associated with the non-standard form, and are happy to be thought of as using
them; women are less happy to use or be thought of as using them.
Interactional Style
There is a stereotypical view of woman as being talkative, chatterboxes,
gossips etc., compared to the strong, silent male. But studies of mixed-sex
conversations show that men talk around twice as much as women. This happens
even in school classrooms, and so is a problem for schools. But when women
contribute as much as 50% of men, they are judged to be "dominating" the
conversation.
Graddol & Swann (see handout) suggest that women are happy to take the lead
in conversation from men who lead it into certain topics, while men are less
likely to respond to cues provided by women for new subjects of discussion.
In their studies, Zimmerman and West found that women were much less likely
to interrupt than men (5% vs. 75%)
Lakoff suggested that women's use of tag questions showed that they were
more uncertain than men; subsequent investigation by e.g. Janet Homes eems
to indicate that in fact tag questions are more geared to involving the other
person - they're not just looking for reassurance - note the number of
"facilitative" tags in the following data:
|
Female |
Male |
express uncertainty |
35% |
61% |
facilitative |
59% |
26% |
softening |
6% |
13% |
confrontational |
0% |
0% |
total samples |
51 |
39 |
The above data (and others) suggest that women are better partners in a
conversation than men.
Key Points (from handout)
- sex-exclusive features
- sex-preferential features
- sex vs. gender
- ineractional style
- use of standard features
- women's style is not (necessarily) tentative, but involves the interlocutor
Language in the Individual and in
Society notes index